Let’s say I wanted to find out what brain regions are responsible for recognizing numbers. I could put people in an fMRI—a kind of brain scan machine—to see which parts of their brain receive blood flow when performing an active task, like hitting a red button when numbers flash on a screen. However, I’d also want to give them a passive task, like fixating on a crosshair floating around the screen, to establish a baseline resting state. That way I could tell the difference between blood flow attributed to recognizing numbers from typical levels of blood flow in the brain.
In 2001, Marcus Raichle, a neurologist at Washington University, ran such an experiment—and he noticed an odd consistency in the brain states of participants in his study. The fact that they were exhibiting similar brain states was not unusual by itself. After all, performing specific tasks means similar regions of the brain will be engaged for each person. However, the unusual thing was that the overlap occurred when participants weren’t really doing anything at all. While establishing a baseline resting state in the scanner, the participant’s brains showed a surprising amount of similarity. It was like discovering that a bunch of random computers all featured a very similar screensaver.
Subsequent studies revealed that the default mode network, as it came to be known, was involved in thoughts about one’s identity, replaying memories, theorizing about the intentions of other people, and thinking about the future. This is your mind when you’re spacing out or daydreaming. In short, when we are not doing much of anything at all, we tend to be weaving the story of our lives.
ADVERTISEMENT
This has significant evolutionary implications. Our ability to conceive of ourselves in relation to others and the world allows us to strategize, predict, and plan against the circumstances that may lead to our downfall, whether that is our social status within the tribe or our actual death. In the book Buddha’s Brain, psychologist Rick Hanson and neuroscientist Richard Mendius explained how one’s ability to construct a self “has been stitched into human DNA by reproductive advantages slowly accumulating across a hundred thousand generations.”
Yet our ability to engage in self-absorbed daydreaming has drawbacks. For one, thinking about preserving who we are can manifest as anxiety. A 2011 study by Harvard researchers Matthew Killingsworth and Daniel Gilbert found that people engage in mind wandering about 47 percent of the time, and it’s also correlated with unhappiness. The study essentially pinged 2250 people at random throughout the day on their phone and asked them what they were doing, how they were feeling, and whether they were thinking about what they were doing or thinking about something else. Regardless of the activity, people reported being less happy when their minds were wandering than when they were not. Further research found that meditation interrupted the default mode network—as do psychedelics. Meanwhile walking in nature was found to interrupt the kind of depressive rumination that is a hallmark of this network.
All this conspired to give the default mode a bad rap. When left to our own devices, the research suggested, we activate our internal worry monsters, creating a fortified wall between ourselves and our experience.
As is often the case when studying the brain, however, other research revealed alternative perspectives. Humanity wouldn’t quite have thrived to this extent if not for its unique ability to engage in selfish mentation. Knowing ourselves allows us to know others.
In The War for Kindness, Stanford psychologist Jamil Zaki described the default mode as the “untethered mind”—the state in which our brains take a mental trip to the past or future. “The more you engage the brain’s untethering system,” wrote Zaki, ”the deeper you can go, and the better you understand what other people think or feel.”
The journalist Michael Easter described another useful feature of the default mode in The Comfort Crisis. He labeled it “unfocused mode,” and explained how the boredom that produces mind wandering is crucial to creativity, a potential survival benefit that prevents us from repeating the same tasks in the same ways all the time. If you’ve ever had a great idea in the shower, this might ring true.
But all this begs the question: Is the default mode the home of neurotic self-obsession or creative and empathic expression?
Easter points to the modern crush of technology as a trap for our attention—and that’s a big part of the problem. We don’t allow ourselves to unplug, to be bored, and to mind wander. “We spend twelve-ish hours a day using digital media,” he told The Daily Beast. “By persisting through boredom, resisting the pull of the attention, and seeing where our mind takes us, we can come to some deeper insights. And I think there are plenty of anecdotes from creatives supporting this.”
There is research that supports this as well. “Twenty percent of the creative ideas of writers and physicists that we studied happened not while they were at work, or even while they were actively pursuing the problem, but when they were doing something else, and their mind naturally wandered to some sort of solution,” Jonathan Schooler, a neuroscientist at UCSB, told The Daily Beast. His research into the link between mind-wandering and creativity suggests that focusing less might ironically contribute to a widening of the potential scope of the solution.
Schooler’s lab also conducted a follow-up study of the link between mind wandering and unhappiness—and discovered a fascinating distinction. On average, they found that people were less happy when mind wandering. However, there’s a catch. “We found that when people were mind wandering about something that they found especially interesting, they were actually happier,” he said. This has led Schooler to distinguish between mind wandering and what he calls “mind wondering.”
“When you’re mind wandering about negative worries and ruminations, that is going to bring you down,” he explained. “But when you’re mind wondering about curious things that you're fascinated about, that actually can be stimulating for you.”
Judson Brewer, director of research and innovation at the Brown University Mindfulness Center, has researched the link between the default mode, addiction, anxiety, and meditation. He isn’t convinced by this rosier portrayal of the brain’s default mode.
Brewer describes activating the default mode as a tendency to get caught up in a contracted state of mind—meaning a mind that is narrowly focused on something while ignoring the sensory information around it. For example, when you have a creative idea your mind often recognizes that brilliance and the default mode is activated. “But that’s not actually about the creative idea itself,” Brewer told The Daily Beast. It’s about you getting caught up in your own ego. You start to claim the creative idea, defend it, doubt it, get paranoid about someone stealing it, etc. The ego actually becomes a hindrance.
From Brewer’s perspective, happiness, curiosity, and creativity all come from a more expansive state of mind, one in which the default mode is quieted, ego is reduced, and we cease to impede the flow of experience with the dam of identity.
Ultimately, the default mode is simply a network of brain regions that lights up and quiets down depending on a vast range of stimuli. Categorizing it as healthy or harmful might be the wrong way to look at it. We’ve all experienced moments of feeling open, immersed, and elated, as well as closed off, removed, and depressed. We’ve all experienced both stimulating curiosity and abject boredom. Despite their different views, Schooler and Brewer would agree that understanding how the default mode works can be key to understanding how these states arise—and, therefore, understanding what really makes us happy.